Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
2010 American Psychological Association
False Fame Prevented: Avoiding Fluency Effects Without Judgmental Correction
Three studies show a way to prevent fluency effects independently of judgmental correction strategies byidentifying and procedurally blocking the sources of fluency variations, which are assumed to beembodied in nature. For verbal stimuli, covert pronunciations are assumed to be the crucial source offluency gains. As a consequence, blocking such pronunciation simulations through a secondary oralmotor task decreased the false-fame effect for repeatedly presented names of actors (Experiment 1) aswell as prevented increases in trust due to repetition for brand names and names of shares in the stockmarket (Experiment 2). Extending this evidence beyond repeated exposure, we demonstrated thatblocking oral motor simulations also prevented fluency effects of word pronunciation on judgments ofhazardousness (Experiment 3). Concerning the realm of judgment correction, this procedural blocking of(biasing) associative processes is a decontamination method not considered before in the literature,because it is independent of exposure control, mood, motivation, and post hoc correction strategies. Thepresent results also have implications for applied issues, such as advertising and investment decisions. Keywords: fluency, judgment, embodiment, judgment correction, decontamination
In their classical study, Jacoby, Kelley, Brown, and Jasechko
did not attribute this feeling of familiarity to the fame of the names
(1989) presented names to participants who were told that the
but rather to the prior exposure (cf. Schwarz & Clore, 1983;
names belonged to people who were not famous. Then, in a
Strack, 1992; Strack & Hannover, 1996; Strack, Martin, &
subsequent test phase, participants received these old names to-
Schwarz, 1988). Moreover, they could well remember the infor-
gether with new names and were asked to judge the fame of these
mation that the names they had just received belonged to people
persons. For participants for whom this test phase followed after a
who were not famous and thus correctly judged these old names as
delay of 24 hr, old names were more likely to be judged famous
being less famous than new names (for which no particular clue to
than were new names. In contrast, for participants for whom the
fame was provided). In contrast, the participants for whom the test
test phase followed immediately after the study phase, old names
phase followed after 24 hr could not recollect anymore which
were less likely to be called famous than were new names.
names had actually been on the study list. Having forgotten the
Why did this pattern occur? Reading the old names in the study
actual source of fluency, they did not discount fluency from their
phase rendered their processing more efficient later in the test
fame judgments but rather used it as a cue to fame (Jacoby &
phase (cf. Jacoby & Dallas, 1981; Jacoby & Whitehouse, 1989;
Kelley, 1987; Jacoby, Kelley, et al., 1989; Jacoby & Whitehouse,
Jacoby, Woloshyn, & Kelley, 1989; Whittlesea, Jacoby, & Girard,
1990). This increased processing fluency triggered a brief positive
This study itself has become famous, being literally the textbook
affect (cf. Harmon-Jones & Allen, 2001; Reber, Winkielman, &
example of how fluency may infiltrate judgments as an experience
Schwarz, 1998; Topolinski, Likowski, Weyers, & Strack, 2009;
and how it can be corrected for (e.g., Clore, 1992). The pervasive
Topolinski & Strack, 2009a, 2009b, 2009d; Winkielman &
impact of fluency was shown for a wide range of judgments that
Cacioppo, 2001; for an integrative review, see Reber, Schwarz, &
exploit the ease of processing a target, whether the fluency
Winkielman, 2004) that was experienced as a feeling of ease that
stemmed from repeated exposure (as just described) or from other
can readily be attributed to familiarity (cf. Bornstein &
sources such as subliminally primed targets (e.g., Jacoby & White-
D’Agostino, 1992; Garcia-Marques, Mackie, Claypool, & Garcia-
house, 1989). Because fluency can be mapped onto many dimen-
Marques, 2004; Whittlesea, 1993). The participants for whom the
sions, the affected judgments range from judgments concerning
test phase followed immediately after the study phase, however,
physical properties of the target (e.g., Jacoby, Allan, Collins, &Larwill, 1988; Whittlesea et al., 1990) to the veridicality of astatement (the truth effect, Bacon, 1979; Hasher, Goldstein, &
Sascha Topolinski and Fritz Strack, Department of Psychology II,
Toppino, 1977; see also Begg, Anas, & Farinacci, 1992; Reber &
University of Wuerzburg, Wuerzburg, Germany.
Schwarz, 1999; Unkelbach, 2007) to preference (Phaf & Rotteveel,
This research was partially funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemein-
2005; Reber, Schwarz, & Winkielman, 2004) and intuition (Topo-
schaft (Research Training Group RTG 1253/1). We thank Friederike Fin-
linski & Strack, 2009a, 2009b, 2009d).
ger, Rebecca Spatz, and Carolin Kempf for their support in running the
In some cases, fluency is a valid cue to the judgmental criterion,
such as in the case of familiarity (e.g., Jacoby & Kelley, 1987;
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Sascha
Jacoby & Whitehouse, 1989; Johnston, Dark, & Jacoby, 1985;
Topolinski, Department of Psychology II, Social Psychology, University ofWuerzburg, Roentgenring 10, 97070 Wuerzburg, Germany. E-mail:
Whittlesea, 1993; Whittlesea et al., 1990; Whittlesea & Williams,
sascha.topolinski@psychologie.uni-wuerzburg.de
2000; for reviews concerning the involved memory systems, see
Rajaram, 1996; Yonelinas, 2002), because prior exposure and
notion of embodiment, we represent stimuli by covertly simulating
fluency are ecologically correlated (e.g., Jacoby & Dallas, 1981).
those sensorimotor processes that are specifically associated with
In other cases, however, fluency is not a valid cue and may have
them (e.g., Barsalou, 1999; Glenberg & Robertson, 2000; Grush,
powerful biasing effects, such as in persuasion (e.g., Allport &
2004; Hommel, Mu¨sseler, Aschersleben, & Prinz, 2001;
Lepkin, 1945; Cacioppo & Petty, 1979; Skurnik, Yoon, Park, &
Niedenthal, 2007; Stu¨rmer, Aschersleben, & Prinz, 2000; Wilson,
Schwarz, 2005; Weaver, Garcia, Schwarz, & Miller, 2007) or
2002). For instance, passive viewing of graspable objects automat-
advertising (Fang, Singh, & Ahluwalia, 2007; Hawkins & Hoch,
ically activates those brain regions that are responsible for actually
1992). Yet in other cases fluency may even cause irrational be-
grasping them (e.g., Craighero, Fadiga, Umilta`, & Rizzolatti, 1996;
havior with substantial economic consequences; for instance, if the
Tucker & Ellis, 1998), or hearing a sound played by a piano
fluency of names of authentic shares in the stock markets influ-
automatically triggers motor activity in the fingers of piano players
ences the prices of these shares (Alter & Oppenheimer, 2006), if
(e.g., Bangert & Altenmu¨ller, 2003; Drost, Rieger, Brass, Gunter,
the fluency of product names determines consumers’ choice
& Prinz, 2005; Haueisen & Kno¨sche, 2001). Likewise, in highly
(Novemsky, Dhar, Schwarz, & Simonson, 2007), or if nutrition
skilled typists, the mere perception of letters triggers the motor
additives with fluent names are rated as less toxic than additives
programs to type them (Beilock & Holt, 2007; Van den Bergh,
with less fluent names (Song & Schwarz, 2009).
Besides excessive relearning of the direction between fluency
We argue that it is the efficiency of these covert stimulus-
and judgmental criterion (Unkelbach, 2007), the only means to
specific sensorimotor simulations that actually drives fluency ef-
prevent the impact of fluency that have been discussed in the
fects. When perceiving a certain stimulus, these automatically
literature are mechanisms of judgmental correction (decontamina-
running covert sensorimotor simulations run more or less fluently
tion, Wilson & Brekke, 1994; debiasing, Larrick, 2004; see also
(Beilock & Holt, 2007; Topolinski & Strack, 2009c; Van den
the General Discussion). For instance, discrediting the informa-
Bergh et al., 1990). An unexpectedly high degree of fluency
tional value of fluency has been found to result in spontaneous
(Hansen, Decheˆne, & Wa¨nke, 2008; Whittlesea & Williams, 2000,
discounting (e.g., Oppenheimer, 2005; Schwarz et al., 1991; for
2001a, 2001b) automatically triggers a subtle positive affect
reviews, see Clore, 1992; Kelley & Rhodes, 2002; Schwarz, 2004).
(Harmon-Jones & Allen, 2001; Reber et al., 1998; Topolinski et
However, judgmental correction for fluency is possible only if
al., 2009; Winkielman & Cacioppo, 2001) that is experienced as a
individuals are aware of it (Hertwig, Herzog, Schooler, & Reimer,
cognitive feeling (e.g., Clore et al., 2001; Schwarz & Clore, 2007)
2008; Strack & Hannover, 1996; Topolinski & Strack, 2009b,
and may bias the eventual judgment (Schwarz, 2002, 2004).
2009d; see also Reber, Wurtz, & Zimmermann, 2004), if an
This is illustrated in the empirical findings concerning letter
alternative source of fluency is provided to attribute it to (e.g.,
perception in highly skilled typists (Van den Bergh et al., 1990).
Hasher et al., 1997; Jacoby, Kelley, et al., 1989; Schwarz & Clore,
There, highly skilled typists were asked to spontaneously indicate
1983), and if individuals know in which direction they should
their liking of letter dyads in a task that did not entail typing any
correct their judgment (Nisbett & Wilson, 1977; Strack, 1992;
letters. It was found that letter combinations that—when actually
Strack & Hannover, 1996). In most cases, these conditions are not
being typed— do not produce motor interference between fingers
met, which prompted Oppenheimer (2008) to write about the
are preferred over letter dyads that do. Apparently, participants had
covertly simulated typing the letters, which led to increased sim-
Thus, the only way to escape the pervasive influences was
ulation fluency for noninterfering compared with interfering letter
considered to be judgmental correction, a strategy that takes place
combinations and elicited a positive affect that drove those pref-
at a later stage of information processing. However, by that time
fluency has already contaminated the judgment. The earlier stages
That this is actually the case was most recently shown by
during which fluency is assumed to infiltrate the judgment were
Beilock and Holt (2007), who engaged typists in a concurrent
not considered a feasible stage of correction. Once fluency was
manual motor task during the same preference-rating task. Obvi-
altered, the judgment was seen to be hopelessly tainted. However,
ously, engaging the manual motor system in a concurrent task
if one succeeded in identifying the exact processes that exhibit
prevents simulations of typing the letters. As a consequence, the
fluency variations, one could attenuate fluency variations and thus
preference bias toward noninterfering letter combinations was
reduce its impact on the judgment from the outset. This could be
attenuated (Beilock & Holt, 2007). Thus, blocking underlying
achieved by identifying the actual source of the fluency gains,
covert sensorimotor simulations attenuated the immediate hedonic
which could then be manipulated or blocked to prevent its impact
on judgments in a way that makes subsequent correction processes
A similar approach was most recently applied to implicit mem-
unnecessary. With this goal in mind, we first outline our theoret-
ory, namely the mere exposure phenomenon (Zajonc, 1968; for a
ical claims and then present three experiments in which fluency
review, see Bornstein, 1989), which is the increased preference
was prevented from influencing judgments without the need for
toward repeatedly encountered stimuli. Topolinski and Strack
(2009c) exploited the notion of an embodied memory by Glenberg(1997) that the past of an object to individuals is the traces of
Embodied Precursors of Fluency
sensorimotor responses they collected with it (see also Cohen,1989; Engelkamp & Zimmer, 1980; Saltz & Donnenwerth-Nolan,
Most recently, new approaches have adopted an embodied ac-
1981; cf. the notion of object affordances, Gibson, 1979) and also
count of fluency by exploring its procedural undercurrents as being
the hedonic consequences of these responses (MacDorman, 1997).
sensorimotor processes (Beilock & Holt, 2007; Topolinski &
More specifically, Topolinski and Strack (2009c) argued that when
Strack, 2009c), which are outlined in the following. Regarding the
stimuli are repeatedly encountered, the sensorimotor responses
specifically associated with them are covertly simulated and run
Experiment 1
more efficiently when executed repeatedly. Applied to the case ofverbal stimuli such as words, the predominant response of pro-
How Not to Become Famous Overnight
nouncing them (Stroop, 1935) is automatically covertly simulatedwhen perceiving them. Because it is known that repeated exposure
As outlined earlier, the classical becoming-famous-overnight
to words increases the fluency of overtly pronouncing them (For-
effect (Jacoby, Kelley, et al., 1989) depends on the increased
ster & Davis, 1984; Savage, Bradley, & Forster, 1990; Scarbor-
fluency of names that were preexposed in a study phase compared
ough, Cortese, & Scarborough, 1977), Topolinski and Strack
with names that were not presented before (Bornstein &
(2009c) argued that also the sensorimotor simulations of pro-
D’Agostino, 1992). The present experiment addresses the question
nouncing words run more fluently with repetition, which may
of whether these fluency gains stem from covert sensorimotor
result in immediate hedonic consequences that drive mere expo-
simulations specifically related to names. Because names as verbal
stimuli are intimately mediated by the oral motor system (e.g.,
Testing this hypothesis, Topolinski and Strack (2009c) blocked
Inoue, Ono, Honda, & Kurabayashi, 2007), we argue that the
stimulus-specific motor simulations during a mere exposure par-
becoming-famous-overnight effect substantially depends on oral
adigm. More specifically, they prevented the oral motor system
motor simulations. To test this, we conceptually replicated the
from simulating by letting participants chew gum (see Experiment
classical setup by Jacoby, Kelley, et al. (1989) and implemented
1) or whisper a task-irrelevant word (see Experiment 2) during
two concurrent motor tasks, of which one should prevent oral and
repeated exposure to neutral visual characters (visual stimuli not
the other should prevent manual motor simulations during study-
mediated by the oral motor system) and to nonsense words (verbal
ing the words. We predicted that the classical effect by Jacoby,
stimuli mediated by the oral motor system). As a consequence,
Kelley, et al., namely the misattribution of increased fluency to
they found mere exposure effects for the visual stimuli but not for
fame, would vanish under the oral motor task but would still be
the words. In contrast, engaging the manual motor system in a
detected under a manual motor task. Importantly, this should occur
concurrent task (that is not related to either verbal or visual
not because participants could more successfully engage in a
stimuli) had no such specific effects but rather yielded mere
correction process but because fluency gains could not be acquired
exposure effects for both visual characters and words. These
findings extend the role of sensorimotor simulations to implicitmemory (Schacter, 1987). The question, however, remains
whether these embodied simulations are also responsible for thebiasing effects of fluency on social-cognitive judgments. Participants.
Fifty (34 female) undergraduate psychology
students participated for course credit. Materials and procedure. Aim of the Present Work
Bollywood actors (e.g., Aishwarya Rai), whose names rangedfrom 10 to 22 letters in length, were used. Each item consisted of
Taken together, the findings by Beilock and Holt (2007) and
a first name and a last name. A pilot study revealed that on
Topolinski and Strack (2009c) provide initial evidence concern-
average, less than 5% of these names were known to German raters
ing the underlying embodied processes that drive the hedonic
(N ϭ 10). After arrival in the lab, participants were told that they
consequences of fluency. In the present approach, we investi-
would participate in a study assessing the cultural impact of the
gate whether sensorimotor simulations are the force driving the
Asian culture and that they were to judge how famous authentic
biasing effects of fluency on social-cognitive judgments. Iden-
Asian actors are in Germany. Additionally, they were told that
tifying the mechanisms underlying fluency (Oppenheimer,
simple movements had to be executed concurrently to this task to
2008), we trace back how embodied mechanisms generate the
experiential cues (Strack, 1992) that may bias judgments and
First, in a study phase, participants received 20 randomly chosen
demonstrate how they can be prevented without the need for
names and were asked to merely read them. Each name was
post hoc judgmental correction processes to debias (Larrick,
presented for 2,000 ms followed by an intertrial interval of 1,000
2004; Schwarz, 2002, 2004) or decontaminate (Wilson &
ms. This was followed by a pause of 60 s during which participants
should simply relax. Then, a test phase followed in which the 20
Because the present approach cannot cover all the fluency
old names from the study phase reappeared together with 20 new
effects found in the literature (as reviewed earlier; see also
names in a random order. Participants were asked to indicate how
Oppenheimer, 2008, for a succinct review), we limited the
famous these names were to them using a Likert scale ranging
present research to the most classical stimulus type found in
from 0 (Not famous at all) to 10 (Very famous). They were also
fluency research, which are simple verbal stimuli, such as
told that the name could repeatedly appear. To engage participants
names. To initially provide evidence that the biasing effects of
in concurrent tasks either allowing a covert motor simulation of
fluency can be neutralized once its underlying mechanisms are
pronouncing the items or not, we had participants engage in either
identified, we chose the repeated exposure of names in judg-
an oral or a manual motor task, respectively. To ensure that our
ments of fame (see Experiment 1) and trustworthiness (see
manipulation did not interfere with the proper encoding of the
Experiment 2) and chose word pronunciation in judgments of
preexposed stimuli in the first place, we implemented these con-
current motor tasks only during the test phase at the time of the
fame rating (cf. Topolinski, in press; Experiment 2).1 In the
al., 1989; see also Cermak, Verfaellie, Butler, & Jacoby, 1993;
manual-motor-task group (n ϭ 25), participants were asked to
Jacoby & Whitehouse, 1989; Jacoby, Woloshyn, & Kelley, 1989).
move a soft foam ball slightly in the left hand while using the
We replicated the finding that repeated exposure of names influ-
computer mouse with the right hand to provide the fame ratings
ences the fluency-based fame judgments of these names (Jacoby,
(cf. Topolinski & Strack, 2009c). In the oral-motor-task group
Kelley, et al., 1989) under a concurrent task that involves a motor
(n ϭ 25), however, participants were asked to eat popcorn during
system that is not associated with these verbal stimuli. However,
the test phase (cf. Topolinski & Strack, 2009c; for other ways to
this effect was destroyed by implementing during the test phase a
engage or block the oral motor system, see Campbell, Rosen,
concurrent motor task that selectively engaged the motor system
Solis-Macias, & White, 1991; Cinan & Tano¨r, 2002; Emerson &
that is most closely related to words, namely the mouth. Note that
Miyake, 2003; Miyake, Emerson, Padilla, & Ahn, 2004; Saeki &
pronouncing the words was actually not necessary to accomplish
Saito, 2004). The procedure took 5–10 min.
the task, and participants were not instructed to do so. We suggestthat, instead, covert simulations of pronouncing the appearing
names were automatically triggered (Stroop, 1935; cf. Beilock &Holt, 2007; Van den Bergh et al., 1990) but were blocked by the
Over the fame ratings in the test phase, a 2 (exposure: old items,
concurrent oral motor task in the test phase. Thus, covert pronun-
new items) ϫ 2 (concurrent motor task: manual, oral) analysis of
ciation of the words may have been trained during the study phase
variance (ANOVA) was run with motor task as a between-subjects
but could not exhibit fluency gains for old compared with new
factor. A main effect of exposure, F(1, 48) ϭ 5.54, p Ͻ .023, 2 ϭ
items during the test phase, which diminished the effect of re-
.10, surfaced, as well as an interaction between exposure and motor
task, F(1, 48) ϭ 4.12, p Ͻ .05, 2 ϭ .08. The conditional means are
How substantially the fluency effect depends on covert oral
displayed in Table 1. Planned comparisons showed that in the
simulation is reflected in the fact that obviously participants in the
manual control group, old names were rated to be more famous
oral group did not switch over to visual fluency as a cue for their
than new names, t(24) ϭ 2.70, p Ͻ .012, d ϭ 0.43. However, in the
judgments. Although their access to the oral motor representation
oral group, no difference between old items and new items was
of the target may have been impaired, their processing of the visual
appearance of the names was not; yet visual fluency was not used
To additionally investigate the possible role of time of judgment
(cf. Topolinski & Strack, 2009c, Experiment 1), which is in
formation, we first calculated the correlations between the value of
contrast to earlier considerations (Jacoby & Hollingshead, 1990;
a given fame judgment and the response latencies for this judg-
ment on a trial level, separately for each condition in the 2 ϫ 2
In contrast to the original study (Jacoby, Kelley, et al., 1989),
design. None of the obtained correlation coefficients exceeded an
this study did not even require participants to wait for 24 hr to
absolute value of r ϭ .05 and was close to significance. Mean
obtain a fluency impact on fame judgments, as was necessary in
response latencies for the fame judgments in the manual group
the classical study. This is easily explained by the fact that we did
were 2,844 ms (SD ϭ 1,411) for new items and 2,872 ms (SD ϭ
not tell participants that the names in the study list were not
1,522) for old items. Moreover, none of the differences in the
famous (because we were not interested in judgmental correction
response latencies for the fame judgments between new and old
processes; cf. Begg et al., 1992; Jacoby, Kelley, et al., 1989). Thus,
items within the oral and the manual group—and for old and new
participants did not engage in a correction process for old items in
items between the oral and manual group—was statistically reli-
which they discounted the experienced fluency but rather used the
able (all ts Ͻ 1). In sum, if (a) response latency and judgment
emerging feeling of fluency for their fame judgments, as it was
value were unrelated and (b) the experimental conditions did not
shown before (e.g., Cermak et al., 1993).
differ in response latency, then it is implausible that time of
The pattern of the present interaction between exposure and
judgment formation may have caused the present pattern.
motor blockade seems to be surprising at first glance (see Table 1)and should be further interpreted at this point, because we also
Discussion
found it in the remaining studies. If asked to speculate about rating
Two decades after publication of the original experiment, the
levels in the false-fame paradigm under a prevention of fluency
present experiment reveals that motor simulations are a substantial
gains, one might predict that all targets, both old and new, receive
causal force underlying the false-fame effect (Jacoby, Kelley, et
ratings as low as the new targets in a control condition withoutfluency prevention, because none of the targets exhibits fluencygains, and thus all of them should be experienced as nonfamous. Incontrast, we found that the judgments under fluency prevention
varied around an average mean similar to that of the prevention
Average Fame Ratings of Actors in Experiment 1 as a Function
group without showing a reliable difference between old and new
items. Fluency prevention did not decrease the overall ratings but
made them simply insensitive to the exposure manipulation.
This finding is easily explained by the fact that participants have
no objective criterion for fluency and therefore experience fluencyvariations not as deviations from a precomputed norm but as
We thank Norbert Schwarz for pointing us to this issue.
fluency differences within the particular items presented (Whit-
Experiment 2
tlesea & Leboe, 2003; Whittlesea & Williams, 2000, 2001a,2001b; see also Kahneman & Miller, 1986), which was impres-
A Broker’s Breakfast
sively demonstrated in recent research (Decheˆne, Stahl, Hansen, &Wa¨hnke, 2009; Hansen & Wa¨nke, 2008; Hansen et al., 2008).
Because processing fluency is readily attributed to any dimen-
Thus, in the control condition new items receive low fame ratings
sion that is asked for, a wide array of judgments can be biased by
because they are contrasted with old items with regard to experi-
it (see introduction). Of course, in many settings fluency is related
enced fluency, and old items receive high fame ratings because
to the judgmental criterion and may thus serve as a rational cue, for
they are contrasted with new items. In the fluency-prevention
instance to judge familiarity (Schwarz, Sanna, Skurnik, & Yoon,
condition, however, the current target cannot be contrasted with
2007). However, judgmental effects of fluency can even reach into
another particularly high or low fluent item, which results in the
domains where fluency is no valid cue for the criterion, for
observed intermediate rating level for all targets.
instance the trust in the veracity of a persuasive message or the
One might object that in the present paradigm the participants in
quality of an advertised product (e.g., Cacioppo & Petty, 1979;
the oral motor group were not affected by fluency because they
Fang et al., 2007; Hawkins & Hoch, 1992; Skurnik et al., 2005;
more likely recollected having seen the name before in the study
Weaver et al., 2007). Even in domains in which judgments should
phase and attributed fluency to this prior episode (cf. remembering
normatively be rational, because wrong decisions may harm indi-
the priming event, Strack, Schwarz, Bless, Ku¨bler, & Wa¨nke,
viduals’ financial well-being, biasing effects of criterion-irrelevant
1993). This, however, is unlikely because the delay between study
processing fluency were found, namely when the pronunciation
and test phase was the same for both the manual and the oral motor
fluency of names of shares in the real-world stock market pre-
group, thus rendering the likelihood of consciously recollecting the
dicted their price developments (Alter & Oppenheimer, 2006; see
study event equal for both groups. Moreover, we know from the
also Novemsky et al., 2007). To address the question of whether
literature that engaging in concurrent oral tasks even impairs
these effects can also be prevented by blocking sensorimotor
memory for words (Gupta & MacWhinney, 1995; for a review, see
responses, we asked participants to rate the trustworthiness of
share names and brand names under concurrent oral and manualmotor tasks, respectively.
Another related alternative interpretation of the present finding
might be that the present body manipulations may have causedprocesses of cognitive tuning (Bless, 2001), which is the notion
that individuals engage in more effortful and elaborated processing
Participants.
Thirty (19 female) nonpsychology students par-
when they are in an analytical rather than a heuristic processing
ticipated for a compensation of €6 ($9 at that time).
style (e.g., Alter, Oppenheimer, Epley, & Eyre, 2007; Kuhl, 2000;
Materials and procedure.
Schwarz, 2002; Strack & Deutsch, 2004; Whittlesea & Price,
of companies in the stock market and of brand names of drugs. The
2001). For instance, Ruder and Bless (2003) found that analytic
names of 36 shares were randomly chosen from the Asian stock
processing style decreased the reliance on fluency in retrieving
market indices Nikkei 225 and CSI 300 (effective 2008), and the
memory contents. It could be possible that the present oral motor
length of the names ranged from seven (e.g., Unitika) to 14 (e.g.,
task compared with the manual motor task induced a more analytic
Zheijanghuahai) letters. The names of 36 drugs (e.g., Duragesic)
processing style in participants, thereby increasing the likelihood
were chosen from the top 200 U.S. brand-name drugs according to
that they corrected their judgments for fluency (Begg et al., 1992;
retail dollars in 2006 (retrieved from www.modernmedicine.com).
Jacoby, Kelley, et al., 1989). This, however, is unlikely because
The length of the drug names ranged from five (e.g., Zyvox) to nine
previous research has shown that, if anything, engaging in oral
motor tasks induces more heuristic information processing (Janis,
The design of Experiment 1 was replicated by implementing
Kaye, & Kirschner, 1965). Furthermore, although analytic com-
concurrent motor tasks only during the test phase. However, the
pared with heuristic processing requires more working memory
following three modifications were made: First, using the financial
capacity (Bless, 2001), it was found that engaging the mouth in
world crisis as a cover story, we told participants that in these days
concurrent motor tasks actually impairs working memory (Wilson,
the trustworthiness of many companies is doubted and that the
2001). Furthermore, analytic processing in general is more time
present experiment assesses the remaining trust of European peo-
consuming than is heuristic processing (Bless, 2001). Our finding
ple in Asian shares and in American brand names. In both the
that the response latencies for judgment formation did not differ
study and the test phases participants received the names and were
between the experimental groups contradicts this possibility. Thus,
asked to indicate how trustworthy the particular name was to them
an inadvertent induction of analytical processing by the oral task
using a Likert scale ranging from 0 (Not at all trustworthy) to 10
(Very trustworthy). Second, the pause between the study and test
In conclusion, the present experiment identified the sensorimo-
phases lasted 30 – 40 min and was filled with several unrelated
tor foundations of fluency-based fame judgments (Jacoby, Kelley,
experimental tasks (watching geometric shapes, evaluating words,
et al., 1989), which are stimulus-related embodied simulations.
evaluating jokes). Third, both concurrent motor tasks were again
Independent of post hoc judgmental correction processes such as
implemented during the test phase exclusively; however, whereas
discounting fluency (for a review, see Schwarz, 2002, 2004), we
the manual task consisted again of slightly moving a ball in the left
prevented fluency from tainting the fame judgments. In the second
hand (n ϭ 15), a different oral motor task was implemented in the
experiment, we extended this finding to another domain, namely
experimental group (n ϭ 15). In this group, participants were
asked to hold a cereal bar in their left hand and eat it while using
a computer mouse with the right hand to judge the appearing
reviews, see Bornstein, 1989; Oppenheimer, 2008; Schwarz,
stimuli. Again, half of the stimuli were randomly chosen to be
2002). However, there are numerous other manipulations that alter
presented during the study phase, and both old and new stimuli
the processing fluency of targets, such as figure– ground contrast
were shown in random order during the test phase. During the
(e.g., Reber & Schwarz, 1999; Unkelbach, 2007) and subliminal
study and test phases, names of shares appeared on some blocks,
priming (Winkielman & Cacioppo, 2001). Regarding the embod-
and brand names of drugs appeared on other blocks, with the
ied representation of verbal stimuli, the fluency induction of the
sequence of blocks randomized. The whole experimental session
pronunciation of names (Song & Schwarz, 2009) seemed highly
relevant and therefore important to address. Most recently, Songand Schwarz (2009) provided an impressive biasing effect of
pronunciation, showing that ostensible food additives with hard-
The 2 (exposure: old items, new items) ϫ 2 (concurrent motor
to-read names were rated as more harmful than those with easy-
task: manual, oral) ϫ 2 (stimulus type: share names, brand names
to-read names. In this case, apparently, the fluency-triggered pos-
of drugs) ANOVA on the trustworthiness judgments in the test
itive affect was used to indicate (low) hazardousness. The present
phase with motor task as a between-subjects factor obtained a main
study should replicate and qualify this effect as being dependent on
effect of exposure, F(1, 28) ϭ 4.74, p Ͻ .038, 2 ϭ .15, as well
as an interaction between exposure and motor task, F(1, 28) ϭ6.26, p Ͻ .018, 2 ϭ .18, and no other effects (all Fs Ͻ 1). The
conditional means for each stimulus type are displayed in Table 2. Participants.
Eighty (56 female) psychology undergraduate
Collapsed over stimulus type, this interaction exhibited the fol-
students participated for course credit.
lowing pattern. In the group executing the manual motor task, old
Materials and procedure.
items were rated as being more trustworthy (M ϭ 3.38, SD ϭ 1.17)
pronounce (e.g., Magnalroxate) and five hard-to-pronounce (e.g.,
than new items (M ϭ 3.01, SD ϭ 1.07), t(14) ϭ 3.12, p Ͻ .008,
Hnegripitrom) names of ostensible food additives provided by
d ϭ 0.34; in the group executing the oral motor task, no reliable
Song and Schwarz (2009). Participants received these names in
difference occurred between old items (M ϭ 3.06, SD ϭ 1.07) and
random order on a computer screen and were asked to judge the
new items (M ϭ 3.08, SD ϭ 1.11), t Ͻ 1.
hazard posed by these different food additives. They provided their
Discussion
judgments on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (Very safe) to 7 (Veryharmful). Most crucially, half of the participants (n ϭ 40) were
The present experiment addressed the biasing effects of fluency
additionally asked to chew gum while judging the names, whereas
on perceived trustworthiness of names of shares and brand names
the other half (n ϭ 40) were asked to knead a soft foam ball in the
and their embodied underlying processes (Alter & Oppenheimer,
left hand while providing their judgments with the right hand. The
2006). Participants for whom the manual motor system was en-
procedure was part of a battery of unrelated experiments and took
gaged (i.e., unrelated to verbal stimuli) reported higher trustwor-
thiness of old compared with new names. In contrast, in partici-pants for whom the oral motor system (which mediates
sensorimotor simulations of verbal stimuli) was engaged, thisfluency effect did not occur. As in Experiment 1, we prevented
Over the harm judgments we conducted a 2 (pronunciation: easy
stimulus-specific sensorimotor simulations from occurring and
to pronounce, hard to pronounce) ϫ 2 (concurrent motor task:
thus prevented fluency effects without a need for judgmental
manual, oral) ANOVA with motor task as a between-subjects
correction. In the last experiment, we wanted to generalize the
factor and found a main effect of pronunciation, F(1, 78) ϭ 12.07,
present claims to yet another judgment and stimulus set, and, most
p Ͻ .001, 2 ϭ .13, as well as an interaction between pronunci-
importantly, to another fluency induction, namely pronunciation.
ation and motor task, F(1, 78) ϭ 8.78, p Ͻ .004, 2 ϭ .10. The
conditional means are displayed in Table 3. Planned comparisons
Experiment 3
revealed that in the group executing the manual motor task, easy-to-pronounce names were rated as less harmful than hard-to-
Mouthfeel
pronounce names, t(39) ϭ 4.88, p Ͻ .001, d ϭ 0.87. However, in
In the first two experiments we focused on the fluency induction
the group executing the oral motor task, we found no such differ-
of repeated exposure, which is a well-established manipulation (for
ence between easy- and hard-to-pronounce names (t Ͻ 0.4).
Table 2Average Trustworthiness Ratings of Share Names and Drug Brand Names in Experiment 2 as aFunction of Concurrent Motor Task
more general terms. Finally, we discuss other forms of fluency in
Average Ratings of Harm of Ostensible Food Additives in
the literature, such as visual fluency (Topolinski, in press). Experiment 3 as a Function of Concurrent Motor TaskApplied Issues: When Brokers Should Chew Gum and Why Advertising Brand Names in Cinemas Is Futile
Our findings have strong implications for applied issues, espe-
cially in the field of persuasion. We tried to ensure a certain degree
of ecological validity both in our stimulus sets, which were au-
thentic names that one can come across in the media, and in ouroral motor tasks, which were laboratory implementations of ev-
eryday behavior such as eating popcorn. Thus, we deem thepresent results to contribute to our understanding of judgmentconstruction in the real world, specifically of repeated exposure in
Discussion
advertising and persuasion (e.g., Allport & Lepkin, 1945;
Using pronunciation rather than repeated exposure as a fluency
Cacioppo & Petty, 1979; Fang et al., 2007; Hawkins & Hoch,
induction, we replicated the finding by Song and Schwarz (2009)
1992; Skurnik et al., 2005; Weaver et al., 2007). Consider movie
that food additives with easy-to-read names are judged as being
trailers or movie credits that repeatedly feature actors’ names. The
less harmful than additives with hard-to-read names. However,
result of the present Experiment 1—namely that eating popcorn
illustrating that this fluency effect is mediated by oral motor
prevents increasing fame during repeated exposure, together with
simulations, we also showed that this effect largely decreases
earlier findings that such motor blockades prevent fluency effects
under a concurrent oral motor task. As strong as the effect of
when applied during the first or second encounter of a stimulus
pronunciation may have been in the manual group, it was substan-
(Topolinski & Strack, 2009c, Experiment 2)—suggests that re-
tially attenuated in the oral group. Without participants’ engaging
peated exposure of actors’ names may not lead to increased fame
in correcting for fluency, we could again neutralize the biasing
or even increased preference (cf. Topolinski & Strack, 2009c)
effects of fluency on judgments of hazardousness.
among popcorn-eating cinema audiences. Similarly, the partici-pants who ate cereal bars in Experiment 2 resembled business
General Discussion
people studying the stock market in the morning paper whilehaving breakfast. Also, they did not develop trust in repeatedly
In the present microprocedural approach to the biasing effects of
encountered brand names and thus were immune to the persuasive
processing fluency on social– cognitive judgments we adopted an
embodied view and identified the sources of fluency in stimulus-
Blocking oral motor simulations may also yield beneficial ef-
specific sensorimotor simulations (cf. Beilock & Holt, 2007;
fects for other persuasive contexts in which fluency biases judg-
Topolinski & Strack, 2009c; Van den Bergh et al., 1990). In the
ments detrimentally. For example, Alter and Oppenheimer (2006)
present case of simple verbal stimuli, which are most widely used
showed that price developments of authentic shares in real-world
in fluency research (Reber, Schwarz, & Winkielman, 2004;
stock market data are influenced by motor fluency; those shares
Schwarz, 2002), we identified covert oral motor simulations as
with names that are easy to pronounce outperformed shares with
being the source of fluency gains driving the biasing effects of
hard-to-pronounce names during the first month on the market.
repeated exposure (e.g., Jacoby, Kelley, et al., 1989) and word
This effect was still detectable at the end of the first year of
pronunciation (Song & Schwarz, 2009) on the exemplary judg-
performance. Although market behavior may also be substantially
ments of fame (see Experiment 1), trustworthiness (see Experi-
influenced by other factors, such as rational considerations, the
ment 2), and hazardousness (see Experiment 3).
impact of fluency on stock choices is apparently not at all rational
Once having located the exact juncture of fluency variations in
(cf. Kahneman, 2003). As simple as it may sound, the present
the stages of processing judgmental targets, we were able to block
findings strongly suggest that both stockbrokers and clients could
selected sources of fluency and thus prevent it from infiltrating the
protect their consumer choices from these irrational biases by
judgment. By this means, we were able to neutralize fluency
simply chewing gum while making investment decisions. The
effects without post hoc judgmental correction processes (Larrick,
present approach also has, besides these applied implications,
2004; Oppenheimer, 2005; Schwarz et al., 1991; Strack, 1992;
important implications for the more general realm of debiasing
Strack & Hannover, 1996). With this approach we expand theo-
(Larrick, 2004; Schwarz et al., 2007) or decontaminating (Wilson
rizing in a long research tradition on judgmental correction pro-
& Brekke, 1994) judgments, as is outlined in the following section.
cesses in social psychology (e.g., Cacioppo & Petty, 1979; Jacoby,Kelley, et al., 1989; Martin, Seta, & Crelia, 1990; Ross, Lepper, &
A New Way of Judgment Decontamination
Hubbard, 1975; Ross, Lepper, Strack, & Steinmetz, 1977; Schwarzet al., 1991; Schwarz & Clore, 1983; Shaffer & Case, 1982;
It has frequently been shown that associative processes can bias
Weaver et al., 2007) by introducing a judgmental decontamination
our judgments by generating experiential information that is easily
procedure not yet considered (Wilson & Brekke, 1994).
available as a basis for judgment but provides no valid judgmental
In the following, we first discuss urgent implications for applied
cue (for an extensive review, see Wilson & Brekke, 1994). These
issues and possible future research and then connect the present
internal cues may be affect, such as moods (Schwarz & Clore,
approach to the literature on judgmental correction processes in
1983; Zillman, Katcher, & Milavsky, 1972; see also Schwarz &
Bohner, 1996), or concepts popping into the mind, for instance
A final way to neutralize biasing effects is mood, because
social categories (e.g., Higgins, Rholes, & Jones, 1977; for a
people seem to be less susceptible to biasing heuristics under
review, see Schwarz & Bless, 1992), but they may also be nonaf-
negative compared with positive mood (e.g., Bless et al., 1996; for
fective cognitive feelings, such as fluency (Clore et al., 2001;
a review, see Clore, Schwarz, & Conway, 1994). However, besides
Schwarz & Clore, 2007). In judgments under uncertainty (Kahne-
the fact that this decontamination strategy requires an affect in-
man, 2003), these proximal cues are readily used as the only
duction, its effects are equivocal in the literature. For instance,
available basis for judgment, which may have detrimental effects
whereas Ruder and Bless (2003) found that negative compared
on judgmental accuracy when they are influenced by factors that
with positive mood decreased the reliance on retrieval fluency,
are unrelated to the veridical judgmental criterion (e.g., Kahne-
Freitas, Azizian, Travers, and Berry (2005) found that an experi-
man, 2003; Wilson & Brekke, 1994).
mentally induced prevention focus (negative state) compared with
In the literature, we find several ways to control for these
promotion focus increases the preference for fluency.
biasing effects—ways that can be divided into pre- and postpro-
Taken together, for all these decontamination strategies the
cessing strategies. Preprocessing ways to achieve decontamination
associative processes leading to judgmental biases remain exper-
focus on the stimulus side and try to prevent biasing influences
imentally impenetrable: For preprocessing strategies they are pre-
from even being encoded, for instance via exposure control (Gil-
vented by controlling the stimulus input, and for postprocessing
bert, 1993; e.g., switching the channel when an advertisement is
strategies they are corrected for by additional processing. As
on, Wilson & Brekke, 1994), changing the presentation format of
Strack (1992, p. 267) put it, “[N]either the accessibility of infor-
stimuli in a way that prevents biasing associative processing (e.g.,
mation nor the particular experience can be undone.” Now we
Gigerenzer & Hoffrage, 1995; Glo¨ckner & Betsch, 2008), gather-
know they can be prevented. The present approach introduces the
ing more ecological information about the to-be-judged criterion to
possibility of a procedural decontamination, which consists of
avoid sampling biases (e.g., Fiedler, 2000; Unkelbach & Plessner,
blocking the associative processes themselves; it is a way to
2008), or preventing fluency gains by changing the stimulus for-
achieve decontamination that requires neither changing the stim-ulus input nor correcting for an internal cue. This provides a new
mat between the study and test phases (Westerman, Miller, &
possibility of judgmental decontamination not yet proposed in the
Lloyd, 2003). However, these strategies can hardly be used once
literature (Wilson & Brekke, 1994) and may open various ap-
one is confronted with a given target.
In contrast, postprocessing strategies to achieve decontamina-
Admittedly, the metacognitive correction dilemma (Wilson &
tion are strategies that take place after processing the to-be-judged
Brekke, 1994), namely the appropriate situational knowledge of
target as well as related information at a time when internal cues
biases and how to correct for them, still cannot be solved by the
emerge and solicit the judgment at hand. These strategies may
present fluency blockade. Procedural decontamination does not
include ignoring the internal cue (e.g., Wyer & Unverzagt, 1985),
encompass the identification of biases and the selection of the most
discounting it from the judgment by some means (Clore, 1992;
effective decontamination strategy; rather, it is a decontamination
Kelley & Rhodes, 2002; Schwarz, 2004), or actively correcting for
strategy itself. Thus, to adaptively implement procedural decon-
it (for reviews, see Holyoak & Gordon, 1983; Schwarz & Bless,
tamination (see also a variety of possible examples in the follow-
1992; Tourangeau & Rasinski, 1988; Wilson & Brekke, 1994).
ing section) the particular bias and its embodied sources must be
However, these judgmental correction processes are time consum-
ing (e.g., Strack, Erber, & Wicklund, 1982) and need cognitivecapacity (e.g., Martin et al., 1990). Furthermore, they requireawareness of the bias and its direction and magnitude (e.g., Martin,
Limitations and Further Research
1986; Strack et al., 1993; Wegener & Petty, 1995), the motivation
The present approach is only the first step toward more exten-
to correct it (Martin et al., 1990; Strack & Hannover, 1996), and
sive research of procedural decontamination in further areas of
the general ability to correct it (Wilson & Brekke, 1994; for
fluency impacts and probably beyond. In the course of this future
reviews, see Strack, 1992; Strack & Hannover, 1996; Wegener &
research, the qualifying factor to derive predictions is not the
Petty, 2001; Wilson & Brekke, 1994).
judgment asked for but rather the target to be judged and the
Moreover, even if these conditions are met, judgmental correc-
various systems processing it. Depending on which sensorimotor
tion sometimes leads to judgments that are still biased, namely in
systems are involved in processing a particular target, specific
the case of overcorrection (Hatvany & Strack, 1980; Shaffer &
ways of manipulating these systems shall be developed. The
Case, 1982; Strack & Hannover, 1996), that is, when a judgment
present manipulations focused on the oral motor system mediating
is recomputed in the opposite direction of the internal cue but too
the processing of verbal stimuli, because nearly all fluency effects
heavily compensates for it, thus yielding a contrast effect (for
in the literature employ this classical stimulus type. However,
reviews, see Mussweiler, 2003; Schwarz & Bless, 1992; Strack &
although the oral motor system is likely to mediate other fluency
Hannover, 1996). Finally, with every subsequent judgment or
effects as well, such as rhyme (McGlone & Tofighbakhsh, 2000)
inference made with contaminated information, it becomes less
or meshing of oral motor programs (Poldrack & Cohen, 1997),
and less possible to identify the discredited information and correct
other fluency phenomena will be mediated by different effector
for it (Ross et al., 1975, 1977; see Forgas, 2001, for the case of
systems, such as the manual motor system involved in the percep-
affect infusion). These multiple constraints of postprocessing cor-
tion of letters (e.g., Beilock & Holt, 2007; Van den Bergh et al.,
rection strategies prompted Wilson and Brekke (1994) to conclude,
1990) and graspable objects (e.g., Gibson, 1979; Tucker & Ellis,
“[W]e are rather pessimistic about people’s ability to avoid or
1998) or the ocular muscle system involved in viewing moving
correct for mental contamination” (p. 120).
Furthermore, the notion of target-specific motor systems being
associative response (e.g., Blair, Ma, & Lenton, 2001; Dasgupta &
the crucial source of fluency is obviously limited to attitude objects
Greenwald, 2001; Wittenbrink, Judd, & Park, 2001)—ways to
that elicit a motor response when processed. Although the range of
procedurally block associative processes that lead to attitude bi-
these objects is surprisingly broad, as converging evidence from
diverse lines of research suggests (e.g., Bangert & Altenmu¨ller,2003; Craighero et al., 1996; Haueisen & Kno¨sche, 2001; Kato et
Conclusion
al., 1999; Niedenthal, Winkielman, Mondillon, & Vermeulen,2009; Tucker & Ellis, 1998; Van den Bergh et al., 1990), there are
The present approach introduces a procedural decontamination
fluency effects grounded not in motor systems but in sensory
of social– cognitive judgments by identifying and experimentally
processes, most obviously in the classical domain of vision
blocking the underlying embodied processes that lead to fluency
(Zajonc, 1968) but also in haptics (Wippich, Mecklenbra¨uker, &
Krisch, 1994). For these phenomena, specific procedural accountsand predictions shall be derived, and how to engage the responsi-
References
ble sensory systems in unrelated processing to neutralize fluency
Allport, F. H., & Lepkin, M. (1945). Wartime rumors of waste and special
impacts shall be tested. For example, can the influence of proto-
privilege: Why some people believe them. Journal of Abnormal and
typicality (Winkielman, Halberstadt, Fazendeiro, & Catty, 2006)
Social Psychology, 40, 3–36.
be prevented by letting the visual system simultaneously maintain
Alter, A. L., & Oppenheimer, D. (2006). Predicting short-term stock
additional unrelated visual information, for instance via concurrent
fluctuations by using processing fluency. Proceedings of the National
visual imagery? To go a step further, can people be made immune
Academy of Sciences, USA, 103, 9369 –9372.
to the effects of symmetry (e.g., Palmer & Hemenway, 1978;
Alter, A. L., Oppenheimer, D. M., Epley, N., & Eyre, R. N. (2007).
Reber, Brun, & Mitterndorfer, 2008) or even beauty (Reber,
Overcoming intuition: Metacognitive difficulty activates analytic rea-
soning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 569 –576.
Also, fluency effects in a given stimulus domain may not be tied
Amodio, D. M. (2009). Intergroup anxiety effects on the control of racial
stereotypes: A psychoneuroendocrine analysis. Journal of Experimental
exclusively to one motor or sensory system. Take, for instance, the
Social Psychology, 45, 60 – 67.
fluency manipulation of the figure– ground contrast of words (e.g.,
Amodio, D. M., Devine, P. G., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2008). Individual
Reber & Schwarz, 1999; Unkelbach, 2007). Although the present
differences in the regulation of intergroup bias: The role of conflict
findings strongly suggest that judgments of repeating names draw
monitoring and neural signals for control. Journal of Personality and
exclusively on motor fluency (see also Topolinski & Strack,
Social Psychology, 94, 60 –74.
2009c, Experiment 1), visual manipulations may not alter the
Bacon, F. T. (1979). Credibility of repeated statements: Memory for trivia.
efficiency of covert pronunciation simulation but rather change the
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5,
fluency in visually processing the word (cf. Kliegl, Nuthmann, &
Engbert, 2006; Reber, Wurtz, & Zimmermann, 2004; Wurtz,
Bangert, M., & Altenmu¨ller, E. O. (2003). Mapping perception to action in
Reber, & Zimmermann, 2008). Thus, visual manipulations of
piano practice: A longitudinal DC-EEG study. BMC Neuroscience, 4,26 – 40.
fluency may be neutralized by means other than motor tasks.
Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain
Finally, the present account is limited to the case of one stimulus
and its fluency. In other fluency phenomena, such as the truth
Begg, I. M., Anas, A., & Farinacci, S. (1992). Dissociation of processes in
effect, in which judgments draw on the fluency of reading state-
belief: Source recollection, statement familiarity, and the illusion of
ments (e.g., Reber & Schwarz, 1999; Unkelbach, 2007; Weaver et
truth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 446 – 458.
al., 2007), or the retrieval heuristic, in which judgments draw on
Beilock, S. L., & Holt, L. E. (2007). Embodied preference judgments: Can
the fluency of retrieving several memory contents (Koriat & Levy-
likeability be driven by the motor system? Psychological Science, 18,
Sadot, 2001; Schwarz et al., 1991), the fluency in processing
involves several stimuli, usually concepts and their semantic rela-
Blair, I. V., Ma, J. E., & Lenton, A. P. (2001). Imagining stereotypes away:
tions. Here, factors other than the mere efficiency of processing
The moderation of implicit stereotypes through mental imagery. Journalof Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 828 – 841.
may play a causal role, for instance the temporal contiguity of the
Bless, H. (2001). The consequences of mood on the processing of social
concepts, which determines how their representations are meshed
information. In A. Tesser & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Blackwell handbook in
(cf. Botvinick & Plaut, 2006; McClelland et al., 1995; Norman &
social psychology (pp. 391– 412). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
O’Reilly, 2003), as was shown most recently by Topolinski and
Bless, H., Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., Golisano, V., Rabe, C., & Wölk, M.
(1996). Mood and the use of scripts: Does happy mood make people
Finally, the notion of a processing-related blockade of associa-
really mindless? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63,
tive mechanisms biasing judgments and attitudes may also be
employed in research on implicit attitudes. Future research may
Bornstein, R. F. (1989). Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis
find— besides top-down control of the associative response (see,
of research, 1968 –1987. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 265–289.
for recent approaches, Amodio, 2009; Amodio, Devine, &
Bornstein, R. F., & D’Agostino, P. R. (1992). Stimulus recognition and the
mere exposure effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63,
Harmon-Jones, 2008; Payne, 2001; Richeson & Trawalter, 2005),
alteration of the associative processes via slow learning mecha-
Botvinick, M., & Plaut, D. C. (2006). Short-term memory for serial order:
nisms (Rydell & McConnell, 2006; for instance via conditioning,
A recurrent neural network model. Psychological Review, 113, 201–233.
Ito, Chiao, Devine, Lorig, & Cacioppo, 2006; or excessive nega-
Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1979). Effects of message repetition and
tion training, Kawakami, Moll, Hermsen, Dovidio, & Russin,
position on cognitive responses, recall, and persuasion. Journal of Per-
2000), or activation of information that is incompatible with the
sonality and Social Psychology, 37, 97–109.
Campbell, R., Rosen, S., Solis-Macias, V., & White, T. (1991). Stress in
ing without instruction: Frequency formats. Psychological Review, 102,
silent reading: Effects of concurrent articulation on the detection of
syllabic stress patterns in written words in English speakers. Language
Gilbert, D. (1993). The assent of man: Mental representation and the
and Cognitive Processes, 6, 29 – 47.
control of belief. In D. M. Wegner & J. W. Pennebaker (Eds.), Hand-
Cermak, L. S., Verfaellie, M., Butler, T., & Jacoby, L. L. (1993). Attribu-
book of mental control (pp. 57– 87). Englewood Cliffs NJ: Prentice–
tions of familiarity in amnesia: Evidence from a fame judgment task. Neuropsychology, 7, 510 –518.
Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What memory is for. Behavioral and Brain
Cinan, S., & Tanör, Ö. Ö. (2002). An attempt to discriminate different
types of executive functions in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Mem-
Glenberg, A. M., & Robertson, D. A. (2000). Symbol grounding and
meaning: A comparison of high-dimensional and embodied theories of
Clore, G. L. (1992). Cognitive phenomenology: Feelings and the construc-
meaning. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 379 – 401.
tion of judgment. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction
Glöckner, A., & Betsch, T. (2008). Multiple-reason decision making based
of social judgment (pp. 133–163). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
on automatic processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learn-
Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., & Conway, M. (1994). Affective causes and
ing, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 1055–1075.
consequences of social information processing. In R. S. Wyer & T. K.
Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: Motor control,
Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 323–
imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 377– 442.
Gupta, P., & MacWhinney, B. (1995). Is the articulatory loop articulatory
Clore, G. L., Wyer, R. S. J., Dienes, B., Gasper, K., Gohm, C., & Isbell, L.
or auditory? Reexamining the effects of concurrent articulation on
(2001). Affective feelings as feedback: Some cognitive consequences. In
immediate serial recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 63– 88.
L. L. Martin & G. L. Clore (Eds.), Theories of mood and cognition: A
Hansen, J., Decheˆne, A., & Wa¨nke, M. (2008). Discrepant fluency in-
user’s guidebook (pp. 27– 62). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
creases subjective truth. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44,
Cohen, R. L. (1989). Memory for action events: The power of enactment.
Hansen, J., & Wa¨nke, M. (2008). It’s the difference that counts: Expect-
Educational Psychology Review, 1, 57– 80.
ancy/experience discrepancy moderates the use of ease of retrieval in
Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Umilta`, C. A., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Evidence
attitude judgments. Social Cognition, 26, 447– 468.
for visuomotor priming effect. NeuroReport, 8, 347–349.
Harmon-Jones, E., & Allen, J. B. (2001). The role of affect in the mere
Dasgupta, N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2001). On the malleability of automatic
exposure effect: Evidence from psychophysiological and individual dif-
attitudes: Combating automatic prejudice with images of admired and
ferences approaches. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27,
disliked individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81,
Hasher, L., Goldstein, D., & Toppino, T. (1977). Frequency and the
Decheˆne, A., Stahl, C., Hansen, J., & Wa¨nke, M. (2009). Mix me a list:
conference of referential validity. Journal of Verbal Learning and Ver-
Context moderates the truth effect and the mere-exposure effect. Journalof Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 1117–1122.
Hatvany, N., & Strack, F. (1980). The impact of a discredited key witness.
Drost, U. C., Rieger, M., Brass, M., Gunter, T. C., & Prinz, W. (2005). Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 10, 490 –509.
When hearing turns into playing: Movement induction by auditory
Haueisen, J., & Knösche, T. R. (2001). Involuntary motor activity in
stimuli in pianists. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Sec-
pianists evoked by music perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuro-tion A: Human Experimental Psychology, 58A, 1376 –1389.
Emerson, M. J., & Miyake, A. (2003). The role of inner speech in task
Hawkins, S. A., & Hoch, S. J. (1992). Low involvement learning: Memory
switching: A dual-task investigation. Journal of Memory and Language,
without evaluation. Journal of Consumer Research, 19, 212–225.
Hertwig, R., Herzog, S. M., Schooler, L. J., & Reimer, T. (2008). Fluency
Engelkamp, J., & Zimmer, H. D. (1980). Clause relations and picture
heuristic: A model of how the mind exploits a by-product of information
viewing. Archiv fu¨r Psychologie, 133, 129 –138.
retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Fang, X., Singh, S., & Ahluwalia, R. (2007). An examination of different
explanations for the mere exposure effect. Journal of Consumer Re-
Higgins, E. T., Rholes, W., & Jones, C. R. (1977). Category accessibility
and impression formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
Fiedler, K. (2000). Beware of samples! A cognitive-ecological sampling
approach to judgment biases. Psychological Review, 107, 659 – 676.
Holyoak, K. J., & Gordon, P. C. (1983). Social reference points. Journal of
Forgas, J. P. (2001). The affect infusion model (AIM): An integrative
Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 881– 887.
theory of mood effects on cognition and judgments. In L. L. Martin &
Hommel, B., Mu¨sseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The
G. L. Clore (Eds.), Theories of mood and cognition (pp. 99 –134).
theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action
planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849 –937.
Forster, K. L., & Davis, C. (1984). Repetition priming and frequency
Inoue, M. S., Ono, T., Honda, E., & Kurabayashi, T. (2007). Characteris-
attenuation in lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
tics of movement of the lips, tongue and velum during a bilabial plosive:
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 680 – 698.
A noninvasive study using a magnetic resonance imaging movie. Angle
Freitas, A. L., Azizian, A., Travers, S., & Berry, S. A. (2005). The
Orthodontist, 77, 612– 618.
evaluative connotation of processing fluency: Inherently positive or
Ito, T. A., Chiao, K. W., Devine, P. G., Lorig, T. S., & Cacioppo, T. (2006).
moderated by motivational context? Journal of Experimental Social
The influence of facial feedback on race bias. Psychological Science, 17,
Garcia-Marques, T., Mackie, D. M., Claypool, H. M., & Garcia-Marques,
Jacoby, L. L., Allan, L. G., Collins, J. C., & Larwill, L. K. (1988). Memory
L. (2004). Positivity can cue familiarity. Personality and Social Psy-
influences subjective experience: Noise judgments. Journal of Experi-chology Bulletin, 30, 585–593. mental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 240 –247.
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston,
Jacoby, L. L., & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobio-
graphical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental
Gigerenzer, G., & Hoffrage, U. (1995). How to improve Bayesian reason-
Psychology: General, 110, 306 –340.
Jacoby, L. L., & Hollingshead, A. (1990). Reading student essays may be
conjointly (?): Rhyme as reason in aphorisms. Psychological Science,
hazardous to your spelling: Effects of reading incorrectly and correctly
spelled words. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 44, 345–358.
Miyake, A., Emerson, M. J., Padilla, F., & Ahn, J. (2004). Inner speech as
Jacoby, L. L., & Kelley, C. M. (1987). Unconscious influences of memory
a retrieval aid for task goals: The effects of cue type and articulatory
for a prior event. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 314 –
suppression in the random task cuing paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 115,
Jacoby, L. L., Kelley, C. M., Brown, J., & Jasechko, J. (1989). Becoming
Mussweiler, T. (2003). Comparison processes in social judgment: Mech-
famous overnight: Limits on the ability to avoid unconscious influences
anisms and consequences. Psychological Review, 110, 472– 489.
of the past. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 326 –338.
Niedenthal, P. M. (2007, May 18). Embodying emotion. Science, 316,
Jacoby, L. L., & Whitehouse, K. (1989). An illusion of memory: False
recognition influenced by unconscious perception. Journal of Experi-
Niedenthal, P. M., Winkielman, P., Mondillon, L., & Vermeulen, N. mental Psychology: General, 118, 126 –135.
(2009). Embodiment of emotional concepts: Evidence from EMG mea-
Jacoby, L. L., Woloshyn, V., & Kelley, C. M. (1989). Becoming famous
sures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 1120 –1136.
without being recognized: Unconscious influences of memory produced
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know:
by dividing attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,
Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.
Norman, K. A., & O’Reilly, R. C. (2003). Modeling hippocampal and
Janis, K. L., Kaye, D., & Kirschner, P. (1965). Facilitating effects of
neocortical contributions to recognition memory: A complementary
‘eating while reading’ on responsiveness to persuasive communications.
learning systems approach. Psychological Review, 110, 611– 646. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 181–186.
Novemsky, N., Dhar, R., Schwarz, N., & Simonson, I. (2007). Preference
Johnston, W. A., Dark, V. J., & Jacoby, L. L. (1985). Perceptual fluency
fluency in consumer choice. Journal of Marketing Research, 44, 347–
and recognition judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learn-ing, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 3–11.
Oppenheimer, D. M. (2005). Consequences of erudite vernacular utilized
irrespective of necessity: Problems with using long words needlessly.
Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping
Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 139 –156.
bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58, 697–720.
Oppenheimer, D. M. (2008). The secret life of fluency. Trends in Cognitive
Kahneman, D., & Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: Comparing reality to
its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93, 136 –153.
Palmer, S. E., & Hemenway, K. (1978). Orientation and symmetry: Effects
Kato, C., Isoda, H., Takehara, Y., Matsuo, K., Moriya, T., & Nakai, T.
of multiple, near, and rotational symmetries. Journal of Experimental
(1999). Involvement of motor cortices in retrieval of kanji studied by
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 691–702.
functional MRI. NeuroReport, 10, 1335–1339.
Payne, B. K. (2001). Prejudice and perception: The role of automatic and
Kawakami, K., Moll, J., Hermsen, S., Dovidio, J. F., & Russin, A. (2000).
controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon. Journal of Personality
Just say no (to stereotyping): Effects of training in the negation of
and Social Psychology, 81, 181–192.
stereotypic associations on stereotype activation. Journal of Personality
Phaf, R. H., & Rotteveel, M. (2005). Affective modulation of recognition
and Social Psychology, 78, 871– 888.
bias. Emotion, 5, 309 –318.
Kelley, C. M., & Rhodes, M. G. (2002). Making sense and nonsense of
Poldrack, R. A., & Cohen, N. J. (1997). Priming of new associations in
experience: Attributions in memory and judgment. Psychology of Learn-
reading time: What is learned? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4,ing and Motivation, 41, 293–320.
Kliegl, R., Nuthmann, A., & Engbert, R. (2006). Tracking the mind during
Rajaram, S. (1996). Perceptual effects on remembering: Recollective pro-
reading: The influence of past, present, and future words on fixation
cesses in picture recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
durations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 12–35. ogy: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 365–377.
Koriat, A., & Levy-Sadot, R. (2001). The combined contributions of the
Reber, R., Brun, M., & Mitterndorfer, K. (2008). The use of heuristics in
cue-familiarity and accessibility heuristics to feelings of knowing. Jour-
intuitive mathematical judgment. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15,nal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27,
Reber, R., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Effects of perceptual fluency on judg-
Kuhl, J. (2000). A functional-design approach to motivation and self-
ments of truth. Consciousness and Cognition, 8, 338 –342.
regulation: The dynamics of personality systems interactions. In M.
Reber, R., Schwarz, N., & Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency and
Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-
aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience?
regulation (pp. 111–169). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 364 –382.
Larrick, R. P. (2004). Debiasing. In D. J. Koehler & N. Harvey (Eds.),
Reber, R., Winkielman, P., & Schwarz, N. (1998). Effects of perceptual
Blackwell handbook of judgment and decision making (pp. 316 –337).
fluency on affective judgments. Psychological Science, 9, 45– 48.
Reber, R., Wurtz, P., & Zimmermann, T. D. (2004). Exploring “fringe”
MacDorman, C. F. (1997). Memory must also mesh affect. Behavioral and
consciousness: The subjective experience of perceptual fluency and its
Brain Sciences, 20, 29 –30.
objective bases. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 47– 60.
Martin, L. L. (1986). Set/reset: Use and disuse of concepts in impression
Richeson, J. A., & Trawalter, S. (2005). Why do interracial interactions
formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 493–504.
impair executive function? A resource depletion account. Journal of
Martin, L. L., Seta, J. J., & Crelia, R. A. (1990). Assimilation and contrast
Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 934 –947.
as a function of people’s willingness and ability to expend effort in
Ross, L., Lepper, M. R., & Hubbard, M. (1975). Perseverance in self-
forming an impression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
perception and social perception: Biased attributional processes in the
debriefing paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32,
McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L., & O’Reilly, R. C. (1995). Why
there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and
Ross, L., Lepper, M. R., Strack, F., & Steinmetz, J. (1977). Social expla-
neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist
nation and social expectation: Effects of real and hypothetical explana-
models of learning and memory. Psychological Review, 102, 419 – 457.
tions on subjective likelihood. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
McGlone, M. S., & Tofighbakhsh, J. (2000). Birds of a feather flock
Ruder, M., & Bless, H. (2003). Mood and the reliance on the ease of
Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of
retrieval heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85,
social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 220 –247.
Strack, F., Erber, R., & Wicklund, R. (1982). Effects of salience and time
Rydell, R. J., & McConnell, A. R. (2006). Understanding implicit and
pressure on ratings of social causality. Journal of Experimental Social
explicit attitude change: A systems of reasoning analysis. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 91, 995–1008.
Strack, F., & Hannover, B. (1996). Awareness of influence as a precon-
Saeki, E., & Saito, S. (2004). Effect of articulatory suppression on task-
dition for implementing correctional goals. In P. Gollwitzer & J. Bargh
switching performance: Implications for models of working memory.
(Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation tobehavior (pp. 579 –596). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Saltz, E., & Donnenwerth-Nolan, S. (1981). Does motoric imagery facil-
Strack, F., Martin, L., & Schwarz, N. (1988). Priming and communication:
itate memory for sentences? A selective interference test. Journal of
Social determinants of information use in judgments of life satisfaction. Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 322–332. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18, 429 – 442.
Savage, G. R., Bradley, D. C., & Forster, K. I. (1990). Word frequency and
Strack, F., Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Ku¨bler, A., & Wa¨nke, M. (1993).
the pronunciation task: The contribution of articulatory fluency. Lan-
Awareness of the influence as a determinant of assimilation vs. contrast. guage and Cognitive Processes, 5, 203–326. European Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 53– 62.
Scarborough, D. L., Cortese, C., & Scarborough, H. S. (1977). Frequency
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions.
and repetition effects in lexical memory. Journal of Experimental Psy-Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 242–248. chology: Human Perception and Performance, 3, 1–17.
Stu¨rmer, B., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2000). Correspondence effects
Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status.
with manual gestures and postures: A study of imitation. Journal ofJournal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cogni-Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26,
Schwarz, N. (2002). Feelings as information: Moods influence judgment
Topolinski, S. (in press). Moving the eye of the beholder: Motor compo-
and processing style. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.),
nents in vision determine aesthetic preference. Psychological Science. Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 534 –
Topolinski, S., Likowski, K. U., Weyers, P., & Strack, F. (2009). The face
547). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
of fluency: Semantic coherence automatically elicits a specific pattern of
Schwarz, N. (2004). Metacognitive experiences in consumer judgment and
facial muscle reactions. Cognition & Emotion, 23, 260 –271.
decision making. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14, 332–348.
Topolinski, S., & Reber, R. (2010). Immediate truth: Temporal contiguity
Schwarz, N., & Bless, H. (1992). Constructing reality and its alternatives:
between a cognitive problem and its solution determines experienced
An inclusion/exclusion model of assimilation and contrast effects in
veracity of the solution. Cognition, 114, 117–122.
social judgment. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction of
Topolinski, S., & Strack, F. (2009a). The analysis of intuition: Processing
social judgments (pp. 217–245). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
fluency and affect in judgements of semantic coherence. Cognition &
Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer-Schatka, H., &
Simons, A. (1991). Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the
Topolinski, S., & Strack, F. (2009b). The architecture of intuition: Fluency
availability heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61,
and affect determine intuitive judgments of semantic and visual coher-
ence, and of grammaticality in artificial grammar learning. Journal of
Schwarz, N., & Bohner, G. (1996). Feelings and their motivational impli-
Experimental Psychology: General, 138, 39 – 63.
cations: Moods and the action sequence. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A.
Topolinski, S., & Strack, F. (2009c). Motormouth: Mere exposure depends
Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motiva-
on stimulus-specific motor simulations. Journal of Experimental Psy-tion to behavior (pp. 119 –145). New York, NY: Guilford Press. chology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 423– 433.
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments
Topolinski, S., & Strack, F. (2009d). Scanning the “fringe” of conscious-
of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states.
ness: What is felt and what is not felt in intuitions about semantic
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.
coherence. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 608 – 618.
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2007). Feelings and phenomenal experiences.
Tourangeau, R., & Rasinski, K. A. (1988). Cognitive processes underlying
In E. T. Higgins & A. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology. A handbook
context effects in attitude measurement. Psychological Bulletin, 103,of basic principles (2nd ed., pp. 385– 407). New York, NY: Guilford
Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and
Schwarz, N., Sanna, L., Skurnik, I., & Yoon, C. (2007). Metacognitive
components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
experiences and the intricacies of setting people straight: Implications
Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830 – 846.
for debiasing and public information campaigns. Advances in Experi-
Unkelbach, C. (2007). Reversing the truth effect: Learning the interpreta-
mental Social Psychology, 39, 127–161.
tion of processing fluency in judgments of truth. Journal of Experimen-
Shaffer, D. R., & Case, T. (1982). On the decision not to testify in one’s
tal Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 219 –230.
own behalf: Effects of withheld evidence, defendant’s sexual prefer-
Unkelbach, C., & Plessner, H. (2008). The sampling trap of intuitive
ences, and juror dogmatism on juridic decisions. Journal of Personality
judgments. In H. Plessner, C. Betsch, & T. Betsch (Eds.), Intuition inand Social Psychology, 42, 333–346. judgment and decision making (pp. 283–294). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Skurnik, I., Yoon, C., Park, D. C., & Schwarz, N. (2005). How warnings
Van den Bergh, O., Vrana, S., & Eelen, P. (1990). Letters from the heart:
about false claims become recommendations. Journal of Consumer
Affective categorization of letter combinations in typists and nontypists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cogni-
Song, H., & Schwarz, N. (2009). If it’s difficult to pronounce, it must be
risky: Fluency, familiarity, and risk perception. Psychological Science,
Weaver, K., Garcia, S. M., Schwarz, N., & Miller, D. T. (2007). Inferring
the popularity of an opinion from its familiarity: A repetitive voice can
Strack, F. (1992). The different routes to social judgments: Experiential
sound like a chorus. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92,
versus informational strategies. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), Theconstruction of social judgments (pp. 249 –275). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Wegener, D. T., & Petty, R. E. (1995). Flexible correction processes in
social judgment: The role of naı¨ve theories in corrections for perceived
Wilson, T. D., & Brekke, N. (1994). Mental contamination and mental
bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 36 –51.
correction: Unwanted influences on judgments and evaluations. Psycho-
Wegener, D. T., & Petty, R. E. (2001). Understanding effects of mood
logical Bulletin, 116, 117–142.
through the elaboration likelihood and flexible correction models. In
Winkielman, P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2001). Mind at ease puts a smile on the
L. L. Martin & G. L. Clore (Eds.), Theories of mood and cognition: A
face: Psychophysiological evidence that processing facilitation leads to
user’s guidebook (pp. 177–210). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
positive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 989 –
Westerman, D. L., Miller, J. K., & Lloyd, M. E. (2003). Change in
perceptual form attenuates the use of the fluency heuristic in recognition.
Winkielman, P., Halberstadt, J., Fazendeiro, T., & Catty, S. (2006). Pro-
Memory & Cognition, 31, 619 – 629.
totypes are attractive because they are easy on the mind. Psychological
Whittlesea, B. W. A. (1993). Illusions of familiarity. Journal of Experi-mental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1235–1253.
Wippich, W., Mecklenbra¨uker, S., & Krisch, S. (1994). Priming effects and
Whittlesea, B. W. A., Jacoby, L. L., & Girard, K. (1990). Illusions of
intuitive judgments. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 53, 63–77.
immediate memory: Evidence of an attributional basis for feelings of
Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (2001). Spontaneous prejudice in
familiarity and perceptual quality. Journal of Memory and Language,
context: Variability in automatically activated attitudes. Journal of Per-sonality and Social Psychology, 81, 815– 827.
Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Leboe, J. P. (2003). Two fluency heuristics (and
Wurtz, P., Reber, R., & Zimmermann, T. D. (2008). The feeling of fluent
how to tell them apart). Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 62–79.
perception: A single experience from multiple asynchronous sources.
Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Price, J. R. (2001). Implicit/explicit memory
Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 171–184.
versus analytic/non-analytic processing: Rethinking the mere exposure
Wyer, R. S., & Unverzagt, W. H. (1985). Effects of instructions to
effect. Memory & Cognition, 29, 234 –246.
disregard information on its subsequent recall and use in making judg-
Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Williams, L. D. (2000). The source of feelings of
ments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 533–549.
familiarity: The discrepancy-attribution hypothesis. Journal of Experi-
Yonelinas, A. P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: A
mental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 547–565.
Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Williams, L. D. (2001a). The discrepancy-
review of 30 years of research. Journal of Memory and Language, 46,
attribution hypothesis: I. The heuristic basis of feelings and familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cogni-
Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology Monographs, 9(2, Pt. 2), 1–27.
Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Williams, L. D. (2001b). The discrepancy-
Zillman, D., Katcher, A. H., & Milavsky, B. (1972). Excitation transfer
attribution hypothesis: II. Expectation, uncertainty, surprise, and feelings
from physical exercise to subsequent aggressive behavior. Journal of
of familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,Experimental Social Psychology, 8, 247–259.
Wilson, M. (2001). The case for sensorimotor coding in working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 44 –57.
Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bul-letin & Review, 9, 625– 636.
Research Abstracts Dose dependency of calcium absorption: a comparison of calcium carbonateCalcium supplementation is recommended as a prophylaxis against bone loss. This study was performed to determine thedose dependency of calcium absorption in an attempt to derive an optimum dose schedule. Using the well-described oralcalcium load technique, we measured the calcium absorption from t
THIS MEETING WAS QUORATE Milton Keynes, Oxfordshire, Berkshire East, Berkshire West & Buckinghamshire (MOBB) Priorities Committee Minutes of meeting held on Wednesday 25 January 2012, Jubilee House, Oxford Business Park South, Oxford, OX4 2LH Present: Committee Members Head of Medicines Management, NHS Bucks Associate Medical Director, Bucks Hosp NHS Trust